Toronto (Canada): One of the abiding lessons from the COVID-19 pandemic is how poorly prepared we were to deal with a crisis of that magnitude. Naturally, politicians have promised to develop better strategies for the future, but so far these promises have not been fulfilled. Perhaps surprisingly, one exception has come from the province of Alberta.
In January, the United Conservative Party (UCP) government established a blue-ribbon panel of experts, chaired by Reform Party founder Preston Manning, to investigate whether there are lessons to be learned from Canada's responses to COVID-19 which will better prepare us to handle future public emergencies. Because the UCP has since been re-elected, and since the Manning committee is expected to release its final report soon, the government is likely to take its findings seriously.
A deeper look:Cynics might anticipate that Manning, as a lifelong conservative, will produce a report that does little more than justify the UCP's actions during the pandemic. But an early leak from the committee suggests that it may be forced to take a deeper look into the way that such policies are constructed.
That leak revealed that when Albertans were asked to suggest how the government should deal with future health crises, they expressed strong support for increased reliance on medical and scientific expertise and decreased dependence on the opinions of politicians. These responses challenge the committee to involve itself in a debate that has long engaged political scientists and economists: Can science tell us what the best public policies are? And the committee will probably soon discover that the answer is no.
The reason for this can be seen using an area of economics called public choice theory. It argues that every public policy decision can be divided into two elements factual and psychological and it concludes that science has little to tell us about the latter. With respect to COVID-19, factual elements include information about how the disease is transmitted, the effect it has on those who contract it and the impact of masks and vaccinations on rates of infection.
Psychological elements include public perception of the harms of infection, the willingness to accept the risk of illness and death and the cost of accepting restrictions on personal freedom. The importance of this distinction is that while reliable, scientific information is often available about the factual elements of policy, the same cannot be said for the psychological elements.